Summary: A new study reveals dogs’ ability to understand words as representations of objects, indicating a deeper level of cognitive processing than previously thought. Researchers used non-invasive EEG to measure dogs’ brain activity, finding distinct patterns when dogs heard a word and were then shown either a matching or mismatching object.
This study challenges the notion that dogs merely associate words with behaviors or objects through temporal contiguity, suggesting they activate memory of an object upon hearing its name. The findings could reshape our understanding of animal cognition and the uniqueness of human language, highlighting that dogs may comprehend more than they outwardly demonstrate.
Key Facts:
- Dogs can activate a memory of an object when they hear its name, suggesting they understand that words stand for specific objects, not just respond to them with learned behavior or associate words with objects based on temporal contiguity.
- Using non-invasive EEG to measure brain activity, researchers observed different brain patterns in dogs when presented with a matching object versus a mismatched one after hearing the object’s name, indicating a deeper understanding of object words.
- The study found that a dog’s ability to understand referential language is generally present in dogs and is not dependent on the size of their object word vocabulary, challenging the idea that only dogs with a large vocabulary of object words can understand referential language.
Source: Cell Press
It’s no surprise that your dog can learn to sit when you say “sit” and come when called. But a study appearing March 22 in the journal Current Biology has made the unexpected discovery that dogs generally also know that certain words “stand for” certain objects. When dogs hear those words, brain activity recordings suggest they activate a matching mental representation in their minds.
“Dogs do not only react with a learned behavior to certain words,” says Marianna Boros of the Department of Ethology at the Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary, one of the paper’s co-first authors.
“They also don’t just associate that word with an object based on temporal contiguity without really understanding the meaning of those words, but they activate a memory of an object when they hear its name.”
Word understanding tests with individuals who do not speak, such as infants and animals, usually require active choice, the researchers say. They’re asked to show or get an object after hearing its name. Very few dogs do well on such tests in the lab, often fetching objects correctly at a rate expected by chance.
The researchers wanted to look closer at dogs’ implicit understanding of object words by measuring brain activity using non-invasive EEG without asking them to act. The idea was that this might offer a more sensitive measure of their understanding of language.
In their studies, they had 18 dog owners say words for toys their dogs knew and then present the objects to them. Sometimes they presented the matching toy, while other times they would present an object that didn’t match. For example, an owner would say, “Zara, look, the ball,” and present the object while the dog’s brain activity was captured on EEG.
The brain recording results showed a different pattern in the brain when the dogs were shown a matching object versus a mismatched one. That’s similar to what researchers have seen in humans and is widely accepted as evidence that they understand the words.
The researchers also found a greater difference in those patterns for words that dogs knew better, offering further support for their understanding of object words. Interestingly, while the researchers thought this ability might depend on having a large vocabulary of object words, their findings showed that it doesn’t.
“Because typical dogs learn instruction words rather than object names, and there are only a handful of dogs with a large vocabulary of object words, we expected that dogs’ capacity for referential understanding of object words will be linked to the number of object words they know; but it wasn’t,” says Lilla Magyari, also of Eötvös Loránd University and University of Stavanger and the other co-first author.
“It doesn’t matter how many object words a dog understands—known words activate mental representations anyway, suggesting that this ability is generally present in dogs and not just in some exceptional individuals who know the names of many objects,” Boros added.
The discovery that dogs as a species may generally have a capacity to understand words in a referential way, just as humans do, might reshape the way scientists think about the uniqueness of how humans use and understand language, the researchers say.
That has important implications for theories and models of language evolution. For dog owners, it’s also an important realization.
“Your dog understands more than he or she shows signs of,” Magyari says. “Dogs are not merely learning a specific behavior to certain words, but they might actually understand the meaning of some individual words as humans do.”
The researchers are now curious to know if this ability to understand referential language is specific to dogs or might be present in other mammals as well. Either way, they want to learn more about how this ability emerged and whether it depends on dogs’ unique experience of living with people. They also want to know why, if dogs understand object words, more of them don’t show it.
About this animal cognition research news
Author: Kristopher Benke
Source: Cell Press
Contact: Kristopher Benke – Cell Press
Image: The image is credited to Neuroscience News
Original Research: The findings will appear in Current Biology