Dogs Detect Human Stress Smell, Affecting Their Mood and Choices

Summary: New research finds that dogs exposed to human stress odors make more ‘pessimistic’ choices, indicating a negative emotional state. The study shows how human stress affects dogs’ learning and perception of rewards.

This understanding can improve the wellbeing of dogs in kennels and enhance training for companion and working dogs. These findings highlight the deep connection between human emotions and canine responses.

Key Facts:

  1. Dogs exposed to human stress odors made slower, more ‘pessimistic’ choices.
  2. Stress odors improved dogs’ learning about food presence or absence.
  3. The study emphasizes the impact of human emotions on canine wellbeing and training.

Source: University of Bristol

Dogs experience emotional contagion from the smell of human stress, leading them to make more ‘pessimistic’ choices, new research finds.  

The University of Bristol-led study, published in Scientific Reports today [22 July], is the first to test how human stress odours affect dogs’ learning and emotional state.  

Evidence in humans suggests that the smell of a stressed person subconsciously affects the emotions and choices made by others around them.

Researchers suggest this ‘pessimistic’ response reflects a negative emotional state and could possibly be a way for the dog to conserve energy and avoid disappointment. Credit: Neuroscience News

Bristol Veterinary School researchers wanted to find out whether dogs also experience changes in their learning and emotional state in response to human stress or relaxation odours.

The team used a test of ‘optimism’ or ‘pessimism’ in animals, which is based on findings that ‘optimistic’ or ‘pessimistic’ choices by people indicate positive or negative emotions, respectively.

The researchers recruited 18 dog-owner partnerships to take part in a series of trials with different human smells present. During the trials, dogs were trained that when a food bowl was placed in one location, it contained a treat, but when placed in another location, it was empty.  

Once a dog learned the difference between these bowl locations, they were faster to approach the location with a treat than the empty location.  Researchers then tested how quickly the dog would approach new, ambiguous bowl locations positioned between the original two.

A quick approach reflected ‘optimism’ about food being present in these ambiguous locations – a marker of a positive emotional state – whilst a slow approach indicated ‘pessimism’ and negative emotion.

These trials were repeated whilst each dog was exposed to either no odour or the odours of sweat and breath samples from humans in either a stressed (arithmetic test) or relaxed (listening to soundscapes) state.

Researchers discovered that the stress smell made dogs slower to approach the ambiguous bowl location nearest the trained location of the empty bowl. An effect that was not seen with the relaxed smell.  

These findings suggest that the stress smell may have increased the dogs’ expectations that this new location contained no food, similar to the nearby empty bowl location.

Researchers suggest this ‘pessimistic’ response reflects a negative emotional state and could possibly be a way for the dog to conserve energy and avoid disappointment.

The team also found that dogs continued to improve their learning about the presence or absence of food in the two trained bowl locations and that they improved faster when the stress smell was present.

Dr Nicola Rooney, Senior Lecturer in Wildlife and Conservation at Bristol Veterinary School and the paper’s lead author explained: “Understanding how human stress affects dogs’ wellbeing is an important consideration for dogs in kennels and when training companion dogs and dogs for working roles such as assistance dogs.

“Dog owners know how attuned their pets are to their emotions, but here we show that even the odour of a stressed, unfamiliar human affects a dog’s emotional state, perception of rewards, and ability to learn. Working dog handlers often describe stress travelling down the lead, but we’ve also shown it can also travel through the air.”

Dr Zoe Parr-Cortes, PhD student at Bristol Veterinary School and primary author and researcher on the project expressed her thanks to everyone involved in the study, especially all the participants and dog owners who took part in the research.

About this stress, emotion, and animal psychology research news

Author: Caroline Clancy
Source: University of Bristol
Contact: Caroline Clancy – University of Bristol
Image: The image is credited to Neuroscience News

Original Research: The findings will be presented in Scientific Reports